December 10, 2009
Plagiarism scandal grows in Iran
October 12, 2009
Analysis of retractions puts spotlight on academia
Nicola Jones
Nature Medicine 15, 1101 (2009)
doi:10.1038/nm1009-1101
Yet although drug companies are often portrayed by the popular press as the source of all evil in biomedical publishing, just 4% of retractions due to misconduct had declared pharmaceutical sponsorship.>>>
September 9, 2009
Peer reviewers satisfied with system : TIMES HIGHER EDUCATION
David Schley
But an international survey of academics states that two thirds are satisfied with the current system for monitoring the quality of scholarly output, and 90 per cent of those who participate as reviewers remain keen to take part.
The findings were published by the charity Sense About Science at the British Science Festival, held at the University of Surrey, on 8 September.
Tracy Brown, the charity’s managing director, said the issue of whether the system was sustainable was a matter of “public as well as scientific interest”.
But while many of the survey’s findings are reassuring, concerns have been raised.
The vast majority of researchers polled say that peer review should detect plagiarism and fraud, but only about one third think it is doing so.
Similarly, while most respondents say that the system should be able to ensure that papers acknowledge any previous work used, only half think it does so effectively.
Despite these issues, participants caution that expecting reviewers to approach manuscripts with suspicion runs counter to the assumption of honesty and the spirit of collaboration in science.
They add that such a tactic would make the task of peer review unmanageable.
Adrian Mulligan, associate director of research and academic relations at Elsevier, said that the launch later this year of Crosscheck, a pan-publisher plagiarism-detection tool, could resolve some of the problems raised.
Given the principle of openness in science, there is a surprisingly strong desire for anonymity from reviewers, with a double-blind process considered to be most effective.
This consensus has been attributed to a desire to protect junior academics asked to review work by more senior colleagues. According to the survey, editors have warned that completely open reviewing reduced the number of people willing to participate and led to “lame” reviews of little value.
Although more than two thirds of the survey’s respondents state that training would be beneficial, Ms Brown said she was hesitant about the peer-review process being professionalised, as it was difficult to see how any qualification could meet the needs of different disciplines.
Instead, she advocated the nurturing of postdoctoral researchers and postgraduate students by more experienced peers, but noted with disappointment that very few reviews were currently undertaken collaboratively with junior colleagues.
A full report is due to be published in November – following peer review.
For more details, see: www.senseaboutscience.org.uk/index.php/site/project
Further survey findings
A third of respondents say they are happy to review up to five papers a year, with a further third happy to review up to ten.
On average, academics decline two papers each year, principally because they are outside their area of expertise, although workload is another frequently cited reason.
The average time taken to review a paper is six hours. However, there is a great deal of variability: one in every 100 participants in the survey claims to have taken more than 100 hours to review their last paper.
August 30, 2009
Self-plagiarism: unintentional, harmless, or fraud?
August 29, 2009
Retractions up tenfold - TIMES HIGHER EDUCATION
August 12, 2009
Perishing Without Publishing - INSIDE HIGHER ED
July 12, 2009
The insider’s guide to plagiarism
Nature Medicine, 707 (2009)
Scientific plagiarism—a problem as serious as fraud—has not received all the attention it deserves.
Reduced budgets are affecting research just as they are every sector of the economy. So, how can struggling scientists increase their chances of securing their share of financial resources in these tough times?Publish, of course!
What? You don’t have the resources to do the experiments? Don’t worry! A little creative writing might be all you need to sail through the financial crisis. Here’s how: use a solid paper as your base; carry out a parallel set of experiments in your favorite model; tweak the data so that the numbers are not identical but remain realistic; and, when you’re ready to write it all up, paraphrase the original paper ad libitum. Last, submit your new manuscript to a modest journal in the hopes that the authors of the paper you used as ‘inspiration’ won’t notice your ‘tribute’ to their work—even though imitation is supposed to be the sincerest form of flattery, their approval of your ‘reworking’ of their paper cannot be guaranteed. If all goes well, getting a couple of these manuscripts under your belt might make all the difference when you apply for that elusive grant.
Does this strategy work? Unfortunately, all too often it does, even though many eyes examine every paper before it ends up on a printed page. And when scrutiny identifies cases of potential plagiarism, serious corrective action doesn’t always take place. Consider a recent report (Science 323, 1293–1294, 2009) in which software tools and manual comparison helped identify cases of suspected plagiarism. When the authors of 163 suspicious studies were contacted, about 30% disavowed misconduct, and over 20% of coauthors claimed no involvement in writing the papers.
>>>
July 10, 2009
The truth will out
Editorial
Fraud in science is difficult to spot immediately, but, as high-profile cases show, it does get found out. Tackling plagiarism is at least becoming an easier fight.
Scientific misconduct comes in many forms. Fabrication lies at one extreme, but plagiarism and 'citation amnesia' are more common. Some have come to question the peer review system, especially following the spectacular cases of Hendrik Schön and Scott Reubens. Schön was a Bell Labs researcher whose organic field-effect transistors exhibited the fractional quantum Hall effect, superconductivity, lasing, you name it. That he didn't keep a lab book or any raw data during his PhD would already constitute bad practice, but then he went on to actually fabricate data. In 2002, a committee found him guilty of scientific misconduct on 16 out of 24 allegations, and at least 21 of his published papers have since been retracted (a new book chronicling Schön's rise and fall is reviewed on p451 of this issue). Reuben's case came to light in March 2009, when 21 of his papers containing faked data were retracted from anaesthesiology journals. Millions of patients have been treated according to his studies of combinations of drugs for pain relief. In many cases, the patients in his clinical trials were made up. >>>
July 8, 2009
Plagiarism, salami slicing, and Lobachevsky
Department of Radiology, Rush North Shore Medical Center,
Skeletal Radiol (2009) 38:1–4, DOI 10.1007/s00256-008-0599-0
Who made me the genius I am today,
Who’s the Professor that made me that way?
One man deserves the credit,
One man deserves the blame,
And Nicolai Ivanovich Lobachevsky is his name.
In one word he told me the secret of success:
Plagiarize! Plagiarize! Plagiarize!
Let no one else’s work evade your eyes.
Only be sure always to call it please “Research.”
Tom Lehrer, “Lobachevsky,” 1953 [1]
July 2, 2009
Dear Plagiarist - INSIDE HIGHER ED
May 29, 2009
How Many Scientists Fabricate And Falsify Research?(ScienceDaily)
May 22, 2009
Plagiarism Sleuths
Science 22 May 2009: Vol. 324. no. 5930, pp. 1004 - 1007
A Texas group is trolling through publications worldwide hunting for signs of duplicated material. The thousands of articles they've flagged online raise questions about standards in publishing—and about the group's own tactics.>>>
March 13, 2009
Plagiarism in the news (CrossRef)
March 10, 2009
Plagiarism and other scientific misconducts
Journal of Cancer Research and Clinical Oncology
K. Höffken and H. Gabbert
When we were young scientists we heard that: ‘‘games authors play’’ and learned that results of scientific work was published by the same authors in different order in different journals. However, the content of the publications differed only slightly from each other (e.g., by omitting one and adding another table or figure) and the conclusions were almost identical.
When we grew older, we encountered affairs of scientific misconduct ranging from copying text from other scientific papers up to faking results.
When we became editors of our journal, we hoped that we would be immune from such assaults. However, we had to learn that each of the above examples of plagiarism and of other scientific misconduct could happen to us. We met double publications, learned that authors sent manuscripts simultaneously to more than one journal or were informed that authors copied and pasted text (as can be seen from the example below).
Original version
Recent technologic advances now make it feasible to better tackle the methodological challenges of detecting EBV in breast cancers. Consequently, a critical next step in understanding this relationship is to apply detection strategies that are sensitive and specific for EBV and able to localize the EBV to particular benign or malignant cells within the tissue. A recent National Cancer Institute recommendation specifies an approach combining real-time quantitative PCR, which allows measurement of the amount of viral DNA in archival tissue samples, with laser capture microdissection to improve localization of viral DNA to benign or malignant components of a tissue sample (90).
Plagiarized version
Recent technological advances now make it feasible to better tackle the methodological challenges of detecting virus in breast cancers. A critical next step in understanding this relationship is to apply detection strategies that are sensitive and specific for virus and able to localize this agent to particular malignant cells within the tissue. A recent National Cancer Institute recommendation specifies an approach combining real-time quantitative PCR, which allows measurement of the amount of viral load in archival tissue samples, with laser capture microdissection to improve localization of viral nucleic acid to benign or malignant components of a tissue sample.
What did we learn from these facts?
1. Science is not immune from fraud, misconduct nor void of bad scientists. Fortunately, these are exemptions!
2. Journals are not protected against these assaults and
3. Even the best prevention system did not exclude that it happened to us and that it will happen again.
What can we do to improve our prevention mechanisms?
1. We count on the readiness and awareness of our readers.
2. We will relentlessly denounce the criminal methods and their originators.
3. We will put the persons on a black list and urge other journals to deny them the right for publication.
Please support us with our efforts. Do not hesitate to inform us about any irregularity, violation or infringement.
J Cancer Res Clin Oncol (2009) 135:327–328
March 9, 2009
Plagiarism in Scientific Publications
Editorial Article
Peter R. Mason
Biomedical Research & Training Institute, Harare, Zimbabwe
J Infect Developing Countries 2009; 3(1):1-4. >>>
March 7, 2009
Combating plagiarism
Nature Photonics 3, 237 (2009)
doi:10.1038/nphoton.2009.48
Accountability of coauthors for scientific misconduct, guest authorship and deliberate or negligent citation plagiarism, highlight the need for accurate author contribution statements.>>>
March 6, 2009
Responding to Possible Plagiarism
- L. Gollogly, H. Momen, Rev. Saude Publica 40, 24 (2006).
- C. White, BMJ 336, 797 (2008).
- M. Errami et al., Bioinformatics 24, 243 (2008).
- Materials and methods are available as supporting material on Science Online.
- J. Lewis, S. Ossowski, J. Hicks, M. Errami, H. R. Garner, Bioinformatics 22, 2298 (2006).
- M. Errami, J. D. Wren, J. M. Hicks, H. R. Garner, Nucleic Acids Res. 35, W12 (2007).
- M. Errami, H. Garner, Nature 451, 397 (2008).
- M. Errami, Z. Sun, T. C. Long, A. C. George, H. R. Garner, Nucleic Acids Res. 37, D921 (2009).
- Journal Citation Reports, ISI Web of Knowledge (Thomson Reuters, Philadelphia, 2008); http://isiwebofknowledge.com/products_tools/analytical/jcr/.
- ISI Web of Knowledge (Thomson Reuters, Philadelphia, 2008); http://isiknowledge.com/.
- Office of Research Integrity (ORI), Managing Allegations of Scientific Misconduct: A Guidance Document for Editors (ORI, U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Rockville, MD, 2000); http://ori.dhhs.gov/documents/masm_2000.pdf.
- We thank D. Trusty for computer administrative support; J. Loadsman as a substantial contributing curator; W. Fisher for useful comments, discussions, and manuscript editing; D. Wu and W. Fisher for assistance in obtaining full text articles; L. Gunn for administrative assistance; and the numerous Déjà vu users who have reported inaccuracies or have alerted us to questionable publications. This work was funded by NIH grant 5R01LM009758-02, the Hudson Foundation, and the P. O'B. Montgomery Distinguished Chair.
2Division of Translational Research, The University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center, 5323 Harry Hines Boulevard, Dallas, TX 75390-9185, USA.
*Author for correspondence. E-mail: harold.garner@utsouthwestern.edu
March 5, 2009
Study finds plenty of apparent plagiarism (Science News)
Yellow highlights aspects of this paper that copy material published in a previous paper — by other authors.
UT Southwestern Medical Center
March 1, 2009
Borrowing words, or claiming them?
Nature Immunology 10, 225 (2009)
doi:10.1038/ni0309-225
Journals are taking steps to stem of the practice of plagiarism.
Have you ever experienced a sense of déjà vu after reading a colleague's manuscript or researching a topic of interest? A paragraph or entire section sounds eerily familiar—too familiar, perhaps, because it is a word-for-word, verbatim (or nearly so, with a few synonyms tossed in) replication of another piece written by different authors. Or maybe a result or hypothesis is claimed to be 'novel' but in fact others have reported such findings and the previous work is not cited. Or the same data are presented in both earlier and subsequent publications from an author, but the later publication fails to acknowledge the fact that the data were included in the earlier work. Are any of these situations acceptable? In fact they are not. All three scenarios represent examples of what can be considered plagiarism.>>>
February 3, 2009
It's Culture, Not Morality - INSIDE HIGHER ED
January 1, 2009
Problems with anti-plagiarism database
Random Posts
Popular Posts
-
This guest post is from Kayhan Kantarlı, a retired professor of physics from the University of Ege in Turkey. He published a first versio...
-
Jeffrey Beall This is a list of questionable, scholarly open-access publishers. I recommend that scholars not do any business with these pu...
-
The Yomiuri Shimbun Turkish national Serkan Anilir, recently stripped of the doctorate he obtained from the University of Tokyo over plagiar...
-
Richard Knox Many online journals are ready to publish bad research in exchange for a credit card number. That's the conclusion o...
-
When Robert Barbato of the E. Philip Saunders College of Business at Rochester Institute of Technology (RIT) heard he was being accused of p...